The Role of Psychotic Disorders in Religious History Considered
Evan D. Murray, M.D., Miles G. Cunningham, M.D., Ph.D., and Bruce H. Price, M.D.
Published Online:1 Oct 2012https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11090214
https://neuro.psychiatryonline.org/doi/full/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11090214
Abstract
The authors have analyzed the religious figures Abraham, Moses, Jesus, and
St. Paul from a behavioral, neurologic, and neuropsychiatric perspective to
determine whether new insights can be achieved about the nature of their
revelations. Analysis reveals that these individuals had experiences that
resemble those now defined as psychotic symptoms, suggesting that their
experiences may have been manifestations of primary or mood disorder-associated
psychotic disorders. The rationale for this proposal is discussed in each case
with a differential diagnosis. Limitations inherent to a retrospective
diagnostic examination are assessed. Social models of psychopathology and group
dynamics are proposed as explanations for how followers were attracted and new
belief systems emerged and were perpetuated. The authors suggest a new DSM
diagnostic subcategory as a way to distinguish this type of psychiatric
presentation. These findings support the possibility that persons with primary
and mood disorder-associated psychotic symptoms have had a monumental influence
on the shaping of Western civilization. It is hoped that these findings will
translate into increased compassion and understanding for persons living with
mental illness.
A man in his late 20s with paranoid schizophrenia explained during a
neurological evaluation that he could read minds and that for years he had heard
voices revealing things about friends and strangers alike. He believed he was
selected by God to provide guidance for mankind. Antipsychotic medications
prescribed by his psychiatrists diminished these abilities and reduced the
voices, and therefore he would not take them. He asked, “How do you know the
voices aren’t real?” “How do you know I am not The Messiah?” He affirmed, “God
and angels talked to people in the Bible.”
Later, we reflected on what he had said. He raised poignant questions that are
rarely discussed in academic medicine. Every day, physicians, nurses,
psychologists, and social workers alike encounter and care for people who
experience psychotic symptoms. About 1% of emergency room visits and 0.5% of all
primary care visits in the United States are related to psychotic symptoms.1,2
As many as 60% of those with schizophrenia have religious grandiose delusions
consisting of believing they are a saint, God, the devil, a prophet, Jesus, or
some other important person.3 Diminished insight about having a mental disorder
is part and parcel of the condition, occurring in 30%–50% of persons with
schizophrenia.4 How do we explain to our patients that their psychotic symptoms
are not supernatural intimations when our civilization recognizes similar
phenomena in revered religious figures? On what basis do we distinguish between
the experiences of psychiatric patients and those of religious figures in
history?
A review of the medical literature revealed little discussion of these specific
issues utilizing modern neuropsychiatric and behavioral neurologic principles.
An examination of the revelation experiences of prominent religious figures was
needed to determine whether new insights could be achieved about their nature
through the application of neuropsychiatric and behavioral neurologic
principles. We undertook this examination with the intent of promoting scholarly
dialogue about the rational limits of human experience and to educate persons
living with mental illness, healthcare providers, and the general public that
persons with psychotic symptoms may have had a considerable influence on the
development of Western civilization. The selection of personalities for analysis
was based on
1) the existence of narratives recounting the individual’s mystical experiences and behaviors;
2) the potential similarity of these experiences to psychiatric phenomena;
3) the high degree of impact their life stories had on
Western civilization in terms of influencing themes found in literature and art,
religious thought and practice, philosophy, concepts of social order, and
jurisprudence. The following is a retrospective diagnostic examination of
Abraham, Moses, Jesus, and St Paul. It is hoped that this investigation will
help translate the veneration, love, and devotion felt by many for these
religious figures into increased compassion and understanding for persons with
mental illness.
Abraham
The Bible is the earliest source of information about the life of Abraham, the
patriarch of Judaism, Christianity, and Islam. The historical existence of
Abraham is the subject of some academic controversy. Our discussion will proceed
on the premise that he was a historical figure. The events occurring during his
lifetime are generally thought to have taken place sometime between 2000 BCE and
1630 BCE, but this is a subject of some debate. He is described as having had
interactive mystical experiences of an auditory and visual nature (see Figure
1), that influenced his behaviors throughout most of his life (see Table 1).
This phenomenology closely resembles that described in the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR).5 Applying the DSM-IV-TR
paradigm, Abraham’s auditory and visual perceptual experiences and behaviors
could be understood as auditory hallucinations (AH), visual hallucinations (VH),
delusions with religious content, and paranoid-type (schizophrenia subtype)
thought content (see Table 1 for examples). These psychiatric features occur
together as a constellation in psychotic disorders of both primary psychiatric
origin and secondary to medical and neurological conditions.5 According to the
DSM-IV-TR, the diagnosis of schizophrenia requires at least two out of five
symptoms from Criterion A and then fulfillment of the five remaining criteria
(see Table 2). Criterion A might theoretically be fulfilled by the presence of
his auditory and visual perceptual experiences. Abraham is not recounted as
having had symptoms that can now be appreciated as disorganization, catatonia,
negative psychiatric symptoms (affective flattening, alogia, or avolition), or
cognitive difficulties such as impaired concentration, attention, or memory. The
lack of detailed information about his life prevents us from understanding
whether he experienced a decline in social or occupational functioning, as
compared with the period before the onset of his perceptual experiences, as
required by Criterion B. Criterion C’s requirement about persistence and
duration of symptoms is fulfilled by the period of 100 years or more during
which he had these experiences. His generally good state of health is indicated
by a purported lifespan of 175 years without mentioned infirmity. Abraham
appeared not to suffer from debilitating depressive- or manic-like symptoms,
thereby diminishing the likelihood of mood disorder associated psychoses, such
as depression with psychotic features, bipolar disorder, or schizoaffective
disorder.
Other potential causes of such experiences need to be explored. The ingestion of
hallucinogenic substances is known to produce mystical experiences. There has
been speculation that plants with psychoactive properties were valued by the
ancient Israelites, but no direct evidence has been uncovered for their actual
use for inducing mystical experiences in this population.6 Another possibility
would be that of epilepsy-induced mystical experiences. Persons with epilepsy
may experience ictal, postictal, or interictal schizophrenia-like symptoms,
which can be indistinguishable from primary psychotic disorders7,8 and occur in
roughly 2%–7% of persons with epilepsy;9 2.2% of temporal lobe-onset seizures
may be associated with religious experiences.10,11
Grandiose and messianic-type delusions are recognized as occurring in
association with complex partial seizure disorders.12 Published cases show ictal
religious experiences to be awe-inspiring or ecstatic, but generally not
successful in imparting detailed or complex information.10,13–17 Postictal
psychosis (PIP) is more common and tends to occur in close proximity to seizure
clusters and can also be associated with a recent exacerbation in seizure
frequency.18 It is estimated to account for a quarter of psychosis in
epilepsy19,20 and occurs in up to 18% of medically intractable focal epilepsy
patients.21,22 Of persons with PIP, up to 25% may have religious delusions.
Only 2% of those who go on to have interictal psychosis have religious
delusions.23,24 Interictal psychosis is otherwise not readily distinguishable
from schizophrenia, but may manifest preservation of affect, fewer negative
symptoms, and, arguably, greater insight. The greater similarities may lay in
positive symptomatology; that is, that of thought disorder, delusions, and
hallucinations.7 Reliable prevalence data are lacking, but it has been proposed
that between 30% and 60% of patients with partial seizures will also have
secondary generalized seizures.25–27
Abraham is not recounted as having had any infirmities that might resemble the
phenomena we now commonly understand to accompany seizures. Specifically, there
are no signs of repetitive behaviors, such as uncontrolled generalized or
partial shaking, orofacial automatisms, stereotyped behavioral changes,
recurrent and consistent auras of fear (although fear did accompany some
episodes), staring spells, loss of consciousness, falling spells, tongue-biting,
or incontinence. His ability to engage in varied dialogue with his
hallucinations would not be very typical of an ictal perceptual change, since
seizures tend toward being stereotyped in nature and not to be so changeable and
interactive.10,13–17,28,29 Most generalized seizures, and, often, complex
partial seizures, are associated with amnesia for the period during and
immediately after a seizure, and persons often have baseline day-to-day
cognitive impairments in memory and executive domains.30,31 There are no
indications that Abraham experienced uncontrolled motor events, amnestic
periods, or cognitive impairments of any kind. A postictal or interictal
psychotic state cannot be excluded, but is not particularly suggested on the
basis of the available information.
The absence of apparent affective, medical, or neurological conditions increases
the possibility that a psychotic disorder could have been present. Schizophrenia
is often accompanied by both disorganized behavior and thought processes that
interfere with life functioning.5 In the case of Abraham and in the others that
follow, disorganization and cognitive impairments are not apparent. Paranoid
schizophrenia (PS), however, is a subtype of schizophrenia that tends to
manifest little or no disorganization, has preserved functional affect, and is
associated with better occupational and social functioning.5
Psychotic disorder, not otherwise specified (PD NOS) is another reasonable
diagnostic alternative. PD NOS includes those persons with psychotic
symptomatology for which there is inadequate or contradictory information or
symptoms that do not meet criteria for any specific psychotic disorder.5
Abraham’s clinical profile would appear to best resemble that of PS or PD NOS,
and perhaps, less likely, an affective disorder-related psychosis. Abraham
stands as the earliest case of a possible psychotic disorder in literature.
Moses
The story of Moses in the Bible is thought to have its setting sometime between
1550 BCE and 1200 BCE.32 The stories about Moses include a great deal of
information about his background, life functioning, beliefs, actions, and
perceptual experiences (see Figure 2). Moses had perceptual experiences and
behaviors that find closest parallel today with the DSM-IV-TR–defined phenomena
of command AHs, VHs, hyperreligiosity, grandiosity, delusions, paranoia,
referential thinking, and phobia (about people viewing his face). (See Table 3
for examples.) Many of these features may occur together in schizophrenia,
affective disorders, and schizoaffective disorder.5 Moses also did not appear to
have any disorganization, catatonia, or negative psychiatric symptoms, or
difficulties with concentration, attention, and memory (see Table 2). Criterion
A for schizophrenia could theoretically be fulfilled by his experiences that
resemble delusions and hallucinations. In fulfillment of Criterion B, Moses’
social and occupational functioning could be said to have declined from that of
a presumably educated member of the Egyptian royal family to having fled
Egyptian society to become a shepherd working on the periphery of the desert in
a foreign land (Exodus 2:15–22). His flight from Egypt occurred before the onset
of AH and VH, thereby suggesting a prodromal decline in functioning before the
onset of psychosis. A prodrome refers to the early symptoms and signs of an
illness that precede the characteristic manifestations of the acute, fully
developed illness. A prodromal period may precede the onset of schizophrenia by
months to up to 10 years in 70% of patients33 and up to 20 years in some
cases.34 The period over which Moses had these experiences was in excess of 40
years, fulfilling Criterion C. His social functioning and leadership skills were
sufficiently intact to have made it less likely that he had periods of
debilitating major depression or florid mania that might have undermined his
effectiveness as a leader. This could fulfill Criterion D by reducing the
likelihood of mood disorder-associated psychosis. It should be noted that the
religious writings attributed to Moses’ authorship, the Pentateuch, could
suggest the presence of an exaggerated urge to write. Such hypergraphia is a
nonspecific finding more commonly associated with mania, hypomania, or mixed
states; however, it is also a feature of schizophrenia and temporal lobe
epilepsy.35–37 Trimble writes that the hypergraphic output of schizophrenic and
epileptic patients is rarely creative. They are often loosely mystical, and both
perseverative and vague in content.37 In contrast to the relative paucity of
poets with schizophrenia or epilepsy, he observes that the number of poets
suggested to have mood disorders are represented in far greater numbers.37
Therefore, mood disorder-associated psychoses remain quite viable in the case of
Moses.
There is no indication in the Bible that Moses experienced metabolic
dysregulations or that he used hallucinogenic intoxicants as an explanation for
his behavioral or perceptual changes. There are also no key features, as
previously mentioned, to implicate epilepsy as a cause of mystical experiences.
He lived a long life, in excess of 100 years, arguing against the presence of
progressive medical or neurological illnesses. The criteria for diagnosis of PS
would be fulfilled by the predominance of delusions and hallucinations in the
absence of disorganization, negative psychiatric symptoms, or cognitive
impairment.
An increased propensity for violence has been observed in some individuals with
PS.38 Moses’ increased propensity for violence could be viewed as corroborative
for a diagnosis of PS. Reasonable diagnostic alternatives might include PD NOS,
bipolar disorder, and schizoaffective disorder. If the first five books of the
bible are credited to Moses’ authorship, then a bipolar disorder or perhaps
schizoaffective disorder would be more compatible with his writing abilities.
Jesus
Jesus is the foundation figure of Christianity, who is thought to have lived
between 7–2 BCE and 26–36 CE. The New Testament (NT) recalls Jesus as having
experienced and shown behavior closely resembling the DSM-IV-TR–defined
phenomena of AHs, VHs, delusions, referential thinking (see Figure 3),
paranoid-type (PS subtype) thought content, and hyperreligiosity (see Table 1).
He also did not appear to have signs or symptoms of disorganization, negative
psychiatric symptoms, cognitive impairment, or debilitating mood disorder
symptoms. NT accounts about Jesus mention no infirmity. In terms of potential
causes of perceptual and behavioral changes, it might be asked whether
starvation and metabolic derangements were present. The hallucinatory-like
experiences that Jesus had in the desert while he fasted for 40 days (Luke
4:1–13) may have been induced by starvation and metabolic derangements. Arguing
against these as explanations for all of his experiences would be that he had
mystical or revelation experiences preceding his fasting in the desert and then
during the period afterward. During these periods, there is no suggestion of
starvation or metabolic derangement. If anything, the stories about Jesus and
his followers suggest that they ate relatively well, as compared with the
followers of his contemporary, John the Baptist (Luke 7:33–34).
Epilepsy-associated psychotic symptoms are possible, but Jesus is not recounted
as having any of the previously-mentioned common hallmarks of epilepsy. A
decline in his occupational and social functioning cannot be established because
of a lack of sufficient information. His experiences appear to have occurred
over the course of at least the year before his death. The absence of physical
maladies or apparent epilepsy leaves primary psychiatric etiologies as more
plausible. As seen with the previous cases, Jesus’ experiences can be
potentially conceptualized within the framework of PS or psychosis NOS. Other
reasonable possibilities might include bipolar and schizoaffective disorders.
There is a 5%–10% lifetime risk of suicide in persons with schizophrenia.39
Suicide is defined as a self-inflicted death with evidence of an intention to
end one’s life. The NT recounts Jesus’ awareness that people intended to kill
him and his taking steps to avoid peril until the time at which he permitted his
apprehension. In advance, he explained to his followers the necessity of his
death as prelude for his return (Matthew 16:21–28; Mark 8:31; John 16:16–28). If
this occurred in the manner described, then Jesus appears to have deliberately
placed himself in circumstances wherein he anticipated his execution. Although
schizophrenia is associated with an increased risk of suicide, this would not be
a typical case. The more common mood-disorder accompaniments of suicide, such as
depression, hopelessness, and social isolation, were not present,40 but other
risk factors, such as age and male gender, were present. Suicide-by-proxy is
described as “any incident in which a suicidal individual causes his or her
death to be carried out by another person.”41,42 There is a potential parallel
of Jesus’ beliefs and behavior leading up to his death to that of one who
premeditates a form of suicide-by-proxy.
St. Paul (Saul of Tarsus)
St. Paul lived during the first century CE. It has been speculated that his
religious experiences resulted from temporal lobe epilepsy.43 We would argue
that it is not necessary to invoke epilepsy as an explanation for these
experiences. St Paul’s mood in his letters ranged from ecstatic to tears of
sorrow, suggesting marked mood swings.44,45 He endorsed an abundance of sublime
auditory and visual perceptual experiences (2 Corinthians 12:2–9) that resemble
grandiose hallucinations with delusional thought content. He manifested
increased religiosity and fears of evil spirits, which resembles paranoia. These
features may occur together, in association with primary and mood
disorder-associated psychotic conditions.
In 2 Corinthians 12:7, St Paul relates “a thorn was given me in the flesh, a
messenger from Satan, to harass me, to keep me from being too elated.” This
thorn has been speculated to be a reference to epilepsy.43 Other theories have
proposed that the thorn was a physical infirmity, the opposition of his fellow
Jews,46 or a harassing demon.47
We propose that he perceived an apparition or voice that he understood to be a
harassing, demonic messenger from Satan. This perception might have afflicted
him with some amount of negative commentary of the type characteristic for
psychotic conditions, resulting in psychological distress.
The complexity of Paul’s interactions in his perceptual experiences weighs
against a seizure ictus as a cause, as does the lack of evidence for more common
epileptic accompaniments, such as repetitive stereotyped behavioral changes and
cognitive symptoms, as previously discussed. Paul does, however, manifest a
number of personality characteristics similar to the interictal personality
traits described by Geshwind,48–50 such as deepened emotions; possibly
circumstantial thought; increased concern with philosophical, moral and
religious issues; increased writing, often on religious or philosophical themes;
and, possibly, hyposexuality (1 Corinthians 7:8–9). These characteristics are
controversial as to their specificity for epilepsy,51,52 with a preponderance of
larger studies not confirming a specific personality type associated with
seizure disorders.51–57 Similar features may also be present in bipolar
disorder5,35,36 and schizophrenia.35,36 As previously mentioned, productive
writing tends to be more strongly associated with mood disorders than psychosis
or epilepsy. This is persuasive toward Paul having a mood disorder, rather than
schizophrenia or epilepsy.
Paul’s religious conversion on the road to Damascus (Acts 9:1–19, 22:6–13,
26:9–16) is an event understood as marked by the acute onset of blindness. This
blindness has been hypothesized to have been postictal in nature43 or
psychogenic.58 There appears to be a lack of clarity as to whether this was
literal visual blindness or metaphorical, since Paul refers to persons outside
his immediate belief system as spiritually blind or having their eyes closed to
spiritual truth (Acts 28:26; Romans 11:8, 11:10; 2 Corinthians 4: 3–5; Ephesians
1:18). Differences in the three most detailed conversion-experience accounts
contribute to this ambiguity. Acts 26:12–18 relates his conversion, during which
a vision of Jesus tasks him to spiritually open the eyes of the people to whom
he will be sent (see Figure 4). In this account, there is no mention of
acute-onset visual loss followed by its restoration. The application of the
blindness metaphor in Acts 26:12–18 may suggest that Paul’s own loss of vision
was equally metaphorical and served as a descriptor of his profound realization
of feeling suddenly bereft of spiritual understanding; that is, realizing his
eyes to be spiritually closed, before the completion of his conversion to the
new religious sect. In such an emotional state, it is speculated that he might
have required encouragement and emotional assistance to reach Damascus. Another
possibility would be that of blindness due to conversion disorder. The absence
of other episodes of visual loss (i.e., lack of event stereotypy), the absence
of features often seen with postictal blindness (a generalized seizure,
anosognosia for deficit, or a gradual return of vision),59 the presence of
complex, mood-congruent auditory–visual experiences resembling hallucinations,
and the possible sudden return of his eyesight with a compassionate touch does
not fit well into a readily discernable neurological pattern of vision loss. His
perceptual experiences, mood variability, grandiose-like symptoms, increased
concerns about religious purity, and paranoia-like symptoms could be viewed as
resembling psychotic spectrum illness (see Table 1). Psychiatric diagnoses that
might encompass his constellation of experiences and manifestations could
include paranoid schizophrenia, psychosis NOS, mood disorder-associated
psychosis, or schizoaffective disorder. Paul’s preserved ability to write and
organize his thoughts would favor a mood disorder-associated explanation for his
religious experiences.
Commentary About Differential Diagnosis
Although Abraham had a revelatory experience during sleep (i.e., the prophecy
foretelling his descendants’ being enslaved in Egypt [Genesis 15:12–16]), and
Paul had visions during the night (Acts 16:9, 18:9); most of the revelations
experienced by these figures are not well explained by sleep phenomena such as
dreams. A host of other conditions might precipitate revelation-like perceptual
experiences (Table 4). Perhaps the foremost of these possibilities would be
postictal and interictal psychotic states, which cannot be entirely excluded,
since convulsions can be absent in some cases, and an absence of description in
the sources does not exclude the possibility of seizures. Fear did occur with a
number of Abraham and Moses’ experiences, raising the prospect of a seizure aura
of fear. Not all of their mystical experiences are recounted as associated with
fear, indicating a lack of seizure-like stereotypy. In the event that
seizure-related psychotic states were etiologic, the pathway to psychosis would
be different, but the premise of psychosis having a formative role in their
revelatory experiences would still be viable.
Nevertheless, based on the available descriptions that mention features bearing
a striking resemblance to psychotic symptoms and the absence of mention of
events resembling overt seizures, it is more parsimonious to explain these
experiences as potentially due to a primary psychiatric condition. The remaining
conditions in Table 4 are not particularly suggested by a medical review of the
source material.
A shared psychotic disorder (folie à deux) is a possibility for each of the
subjects discussed. The essential feature of this condition is that of a
delusion that develops in an individual who is involved in a close relationship
with another person, sometimes termed the “inducer” or “primary case,” who
already has a psychotic disorder with prominent delusions. The individual
completely or partly comes to share the delusional beliefs of the primary
case.5,60 In our subjects, there are no known close associates having an
equivalent spectrum and magnitude of symptoms who might serve the role of a
primary case. Conversely, each of our subjects might also have theoretically
served as a primary case in a shared psychotic-disorder relationship. Among
other psychiatric explanations, which should be mentioned for completeness, are
those of mystical experiences occurring as a result of unconscious forces.61
These may manifest by way of conversion disorder or a dissociative condition
such as a trance-like state or dissociative identity disorder (DID). Another
possibility would be that of a deliberate misrepresentation of supernatural
revelation. This hypothesis would require significant interpolation to support.
An argument against this would be that the sources used appear entirely sincere
about their belief in the divine origin of these experiences. A very complex
state of affairs would be that of a psychotic disorder comorbid with deliberate
misrepresentations, conversion symptoms, dissociative trance-like states, and
DID. These hypotheses are mentioned to ensure an adequate appraisal of the
possibilities despite an acknowledged inability to substantiate any of these
processes being at work.
Limitations of the Analysis
The sources relied upon to derive information about our subjects are not medical
records. The modern reader faces challenges interpreting events far removed from
our own time and culture and which are recounted in a different language by
authors who had their own biases. It might be supposed that our subjects can
only be understood in the context of these factors and according to the norms of
religious experience of their day. Others might advise that these religious
writings should only be interpreted nonliterally, that is, metaphorically and
abstractly, thereby eliminating the possibility of medical scrutiny.
Still others might propose that medical knowledge is always in a state of flux,
which renders impermanent this type of analysis because of its dependence on the
prevailing medical vogue of the day.62 Retrospective diagnosis may also be
asserted to be a transgression of medical principles, since a medical opinion is
rendered on a patient who was never seen or examined.62
Starting with the last point, since it would offer an obstacle to any
prerequisite ability to derive information about persons not present for
examination, it is our view that it is an oversimplification to state that all
diagnosis must ethically and methodologically rest upon having the patient
present in person. The behavioral and neurological fields of medicine frequently
rely upon the observations of family, friends, and associates of our patients
because of patients’ sometime reticence to talk about their symptoms,
insufficiency of information, or inaccuracies of self-reporting. Samples of a
patient’s writing are frequently used to assess movement,63 thought content,
language function, organization, and ability to abstract. Moreover, advances in
understanding human physiology have afforded us some confidence for interpreting
certain types of findings, such as those attributed to the 6th century BCE
Indian physician Shushruta, who described sweet urine.64 We need not have been
present to recognize a probable case of diabetes mellitus. Similarly, other
conditions may bear sufficient signs through their descriptions in literature to
allow discussion about their causes. The figures we have discussed have
information to draw upon, some of which is held by religious tradition to be
authored by the individuals (Moses and Paul), and some authored by close
associates (of Jesus and Paul), who have potentially provided more information
about them than available for most persons born before the present age. We
recognize an important limitation inasmuch as we approach these source documents
as most likely being composites of the perspectives and beliefs of authors, most
of whom would not have personally known our subjects.65,66 This would be similar
to other ancient writers who related stories about people and events that they
were not present to witness. This analysis depends upon our sources’ reflecting,
in some measure, actual people and events as they were.
The reader is then reminded that the medical interview and examination are not
fixed in any general way. It is understood that a physician must adjust both
interview and examination according to purpose and circumstance.67,68 A
psychiatrist may restrict examination to discussion about relevant feelings and
perceptions, never physically touching the patient, but still reliably deducing
level of alertness, orientation, attention, ability to self-regulate and
perceive social cues, thought organization, language abilities, memory, insight,
judgment, and general intelligence, among other things. A surgeon may emphasize
the need to palpate a mass to determine its characteristics. These approaches
are very different and are adjusted according to specific goals. The paradigm
for understanding behavior in terms of a brain–behavior relationship is largely
a product of the last century of brain research, and it was not available to our
predecessors.69 It is a useful exercise to apply our modern models of how
behavior correlates to neural anatomy to test limits for localization and
diagnosis while recognizing and offering scrutiny to their limitations.
We can briefly outline an example of how an understanding of modern functional
neuroanatomy can allow a previously unattainable degree of discernment about an
individual not present for examination. Setting aside questions about
authenticity of authorship and actuality of events, we will look at what might
be determined about St. Paul, ostensibly from his own hand. His writings
indicate that he traveled extensively, even surviving many physical hardships (2
Corinthians 11:23–27). These activities would minimally necessitate generally
intact motor function, sensation, and coordination. These abilities are now
known to be accomplished through the action of specific brain
cortical–subcortical circuits (in frontal and parietal lobes) that are linked in
a type of parallel processing and modulated by the cerebellum. The
cortical–subcortical circuits are composed of their cortical target regions, and
have as their constituents the striatum (caudate, putamen, ventral striatum),
globus pallidus, and substantial nigra, and thalamus, which then projects back
to the cortex.70 These systems would appear to be grossly intact, to endure such
extended travel and hardship. Paul’s detailed recollections of events and his
references to the contents of previous epistles advocate for intact memory
systems, both semantic and episodic, at the time of his writing. These memory
systems are now understood to be reliant on a deep-brain circuitry consisting of
the hippocampus, fornix, mamillary body, mamillothalamic tract, anterior nucleus
of the thalamus and cingulated gyrus (circuit of Papez). Paul spoke to many
people on his journeys, indicating adequate function of language, which is
dependent on perisylvian cerebral structures. His ability to form social
relationships and speak persuasively using some metaphorical language suggests
relatively preserved basic functions of frontal lobe and limbic systems, which
oversee self-regulation, emotions, abstraction, ability to organize thoughts,
and focus attention. His ability to maintain wakefulness, speak, eat, and
breathe supports an adequately functioning brainstem. For circumscribed
purposes, this examination can be more enlightening than that of a
1st-century-trained physician sitting in the room with St Paul or that of a
21st-century physician without the appropriate training. More nuanced assessment
of cognition and behavior might be achieved via application of neuropsychiatric
and behavioral neurological approaches that utilize modern understanding about
patterns of neurological and psychiatric illnesses. The preceding inventory of
St. Paul’s generally intact neural systems can be largely extended to each of
our subjects. This would be the first such inventory supporting a general
preservation of such neural systems for Abraham, Moses, Jesus, and St Paul. It
is acknowledged that medical analyses are, in the end, susceptible to being
incorrect or incomplete and should always be open to revision when new
information becomes available. Without academic scrutiny of methods and
conclusions, improvements in understanding cannot be achieved.
We now turn to several lines of reasoning that favor the credibility of our
proposals.
First, schizophrenia research has yielded compelling evidence to support the
model for genetic vulnerability, coupled with environmental and psychosocial
stressors, the so-called diathesis–stress model, as a mechanism by which
schizophrenia occurs.71–73
Cross-cultural clinical characteristics,74 an increased risk of having the
disorder according to degree of kinship to those affected, a host of identified
genes affecting risk for developing schizophrenia, and an increased prevalence
of subtle brain developmental abnormalities in persons with schizophrenia70,75
suggest interactions between genetic and environmental influences75,76 that, so
far as is known, would not be expected to have been different in persons living
in the ancient world.
A second point hinges upon recognizing psychosis in the pages of ancient
writings. Psychosis has been known by many names throughout history. Only
recently has more precise nomenclature been developed. It should be no surprise
that we would have difficulties recognizing cases in ancient writings when
gazing through the eyes of ancient authors. Modern understanding about psychosis
holds that a central feature is thought processes reflecting a highly distorted
view of reality or a complete loss of contact with reality. DSM-IV-TR indicates
that the term “psychotic” refers to a constellation of symptoms that may vary to
some extent across diagnostic categories, but it generally refers to delusions,
any prominent hallucinations, disorganized speech, or disorganized or catatonic
behavior.5 A delusion is a false belief based on incorrect inference about
external reality that is firmly sustained despite what almost everyone else
believes and despite what constitutes incontrovertible and obvious proof or
evidence to the contrary.
The belief is not one ordinarily accepted by other members of the person’s
culture or subculture.5 Delusional conviction occurs on a continuum.5 The
depictions of our subjects indicate that they may have either found their own
experiences not entirely believable to themselves, understood that their
experiences would be hard for others to believe, or that they were perceived by
their contemporaries as being mad. Genesis: 12–20 recounts that God made
promises of blessings, progeny, and land to Abraham. Despite these assurances,
Abraham’s fear of death prompted him to surrender his wife to the affection of
kings on two occasions in order to forestall his own execution. Moses, for his
part, points out to God in Exodus 4:1 that the message he was given to bring to
the Israelites stood a good chance of not being believed by a people whom we
would now characterize as polytheistic,77 superstitious, and therefore more
likely to have accepted such happenings. Their rapid reversion to previous
religious practices despite a series of miracles (Exodus 32) appears to support
Moses’ concern. Mark 3:21 confirms an occasion where Jesus’ friends and family
viewed him as mad or “beside himself.” It is intrinsic to his narrative that the
people of his hometown (Mark 6:1–6) and the religious authorities of the day
also did not accept his message. St Paul’s contemporary Festus, the local Roman
governor of Judea, in Acts 26:24 exclaimed that Paul appeared “mad” or not sane.
These events are closest in time to our subjects and might suggest psychotic
type thought processes.
A third point speaks to the concern that religious and cultural factors of the
day need to be taken into account. DSM-IV-TR recognizes that visual and auditory
hallucinations with a religious content may be a part of normal religious
experience in some cultures.5 Rediger observes that there is a tendency of the
Western mind-set to pathologize spiritual experiences and that there may be an
overdependence of psychological interpretation on material existence.78 He
suggests that there is an anosognosia for experiential phenomena that exist
outside the narrow band of consciousness that psychology apprehends and that
there is a great deal to learn from Eastern traditions in this regard.78 He
recommends an approach that correlates phenomena falling outside the
psychological paradigm with medical science rather than pathology. In harmony
with this perspective is the recognition that some spiritual experiences can
have very beneficial effects for transforming the lives of some individuals,
allowing them to surmount obstacles and change destructive behaviors. In
response to these thoughts, we emphasize that our intent is not to prove that
the experiences of our subjects could not have resulted from normal religious
experiences in the context of cultural factors; it is to apply a modern
neurobehavioral paradigm to the experiences of our subjects and thereby advance
a dialogue about the rational limits of perceptual experience. Toward this, we
point out that their experiences, if they occurred as narrated, might also be
conceptualized as psychotic spectrum because of their resemblances, by way of
their recurrent nature, intensity, subject matter, grandiose-like qualities, and
similarity to psychotic auditory-visual phenomena.
It is recognized that the content of schizophrenic delusions and hallucinations
is significantly influenced by sociocultural background. Different cultural
experiences can result in different delusional form and content.79,80 The
DSM-IV-TR criteria presume our ability to distinguish psychotic phenomena from
other normal experiences in the context of a given culture. Unfortunately,
evidence-based algorithms for accomplishing this are not available. Further
complicating our task is the gulf of time over which we must work. Overcoming
these obstacles in some measure might again be accomplished through drawing on
the perspectives of persons closer in time and culture to our subjects.
The earliest believers found the experiences of the subjects sufficiently
removed from the sphere of normal life so as to be understood as a product of a
highly unusual relationship with a divine force.66 Those who did not believe may
have had various reasons, some of which would have been that the message was too
far from their reality to be accepted.
From today’s vantage-point, if our subjects’ experiences had resulted only from
a convergence of normal individual and community religious experiences, we
should have expected numerous such stories and therefore no reason to take
notice of these now because of their mundane nature. Surviving literature,
whether of Classical Greek, Roman period, or biblical origin, does not provide
support that it was commonplace in the ancient world for the general population
to have recurrent auditory-visual experiences as grand in scope as those of our
subjects. The populations of the earliest followers of such new belief-systems,
as those of our subjects, would constitute small groups able to accept the
beliefs before the emergence of social pressures related to larger group
dynamics. How do we explain the existence of the earliest followers? Their
presence would not be expected from an association with individuals having a
highly distorted view of reality.
Social models of psychopathology may be useful for understanding how this might
have happened. Social-distance theory and communications-disorder theory suggest
that differentiating sanity from psychosis can be achieved, based on the degree
to which beliefs hamper or facilitate communication and acceptance by society.
Those who deviate excessively from the societal norms do not relate to the
populace, are not understood, become socially isolated and stigmatized, and may
be identified as not sane.81 This point of view might define as sane any person
who is able to maintain acceptance and communication with a social group. Not
accounted for by this theory are individuals who appear to demonstrate sustained
paranoid, grandiose, messianic-type delusions, who, in more modern times, have
drawn numbers of adherents. Two such individuals are David Koresh, of the Branch
Dravidians,82 and Marshall Applewhite, of the Heaven’s Gate cult.83 There are
others in recent times who have claimed to be prophet, messiah,84 Jesus,85
Buddha,86 avatar,87 or madhi,88 who have acquired followings. If David Koresh
and Marshall Applewhite are appreciated as having psychotic-spectrum beliefs,
then the premise becomes untenable that the diagnosis of psychosis must rigidly
rely upon an inability to maintain a social group. A subset of individuals with
psychotic symptoms appears able to form intense social bonds and communities
despite having an extremely distorted view of reality. The existence of a better
socially functioning subset of individuals with psychotic-type symptoms is
corroborated by research indicating that psychotic-like experiences, including
both bizarre and non-bizarre delusion-like beliefs, are frequently found in the
general population. This supports the idea that psychotic symptoms likely lie on
a continuum.89–92
Political-psychology models of leader–follower relationships may provide useful
insights as to how early followers could have coalesced around our subjects.
Wilner93 surveyed the literature on the topic of charismatic leadership and
defined it as a relationship between a leader and a group of followers, having
the following properties:
1.
The leader is perceived by the followers as somehow superhuman.
2.
The followers blindly believe the leader’s statements.
3.
The followers unconditionally comply with the leader’s directives for action.
4.
The followers give the leader unqualified emotional support.
Also, Wilner identifies four “catalytic factors” that are shared by charismatic
leaders. The first factor is the assimilation of a leader to one or more of the
dominant myths of his society or culture. The second is the performance of what
appears to be an extraordinary or heroic feat. The third is the projection of
the possession of qualities with an uncanny or a powerful aura. Finally, there
is outstanding rhetorical ability”93.
It is reasonable to speculate that a charismatic leadership–follower group
dynamic was present between our subjects and their followers. Little further
comment can be made about Abraham in this regard since so little information is
available about him. Moses felt himself not to be a good speaker, and his
brother Aaron was appointed to speak on his behalf to the community (Exodus
4:10–16). This raises interesting questions about what roles community members
might contribute to the functioning of a leadership–follower dynamic in order to
supplement the leader’s deficiencies. The narratives of Jesus and Paul have
details which could fit into a charismatic leader–follower paradigm of group
behavior.
Creating and sustaining groups would be dependent on additional mechanisms:
Wilfred Bion94 observed three patterns of group behavior that occur in healthy,
mature adults, wherein group members act as if they are dominated: the
dependency group, the pairing group, and the fight–flight group. The dependency
group turns to an omnipotent leader for security, behaving as if they do not
have independent minds of their own. Members blindly seek directions and follow
orders unquestioningly. They tend to idealize and place the leader on a
pedestal. When the leader fails to meet the standards of omnipotence and
omniscience, a period of denial, then anger, and disappointment result. In the
pairing group, the members act as if the goal of the group is to bring forth a
messiah, someone who will save them. There is an air of optimism and hope that a
new world is around the corner. The fight–flight group organizes itself in
relationship to a perceived outside threat. The group itself is idealized as
part of a polarizing mechanism, while the outside population is regularly seen
as malevolent in motivation. The threatening outside world is at once a threat
to the existence of the group and the justification for its existence. Each of
these group types regularly characterizes the followers in charismatic
leader–follower relationships.95 It is reasonable to propose that one or more of
these types of group dynamics were present to varying degrees, whether
simultaneously or in various sequences, in our subjects’ groups as they
developed their beliefs over time.
How do individuals with mental illness rise to positions of leadership? Ghaemi96
sets forth a hypothesis that there are key elements associated with mental
illness that may be beneficial for leadership abilities: realism, resilience,
empathy, and creativity.96 His analysis of several notable political, military
and business leaders and review of psychological research leads to his proposal
that depression can be associated with an increase in each of these qualities,
and mania can be associated with an enhancement of creativity and resilience.
Depression promotes leaders’ being more realistic and empathic, whereas mania
promotes their being more creative and resilient.96 He adds that when depression
and mania occur together in bipolar disorder, it may result in a further
increase in leadership skills. Such individuals, he proposes, benefit indirectly
from entering and leaving these mood states in addition to being in their well
state between episodes.96 If this were to hold true, then our subjects might be
more likely to have affective disorder-associated psychotic conditions and
thereby could have benefitted from spending periods in various mood states,
including their well states. The quality of realism would be expected to be most
adversely affected in a psychotic state, especially when judged from the
standpoint of modern sensibilities. With respect to religious beliefs arising
during historical periods preceding the advent of increased understanding about
the natural world, there might have been less by which to judge this quality
and, therefore, more cultural tolerance or acceptance of a wider range of ideas.
A shared psychotic disorder5,60 is another means by which the earliest followers
may have received their beliefs, with each of our subjects being a primary case.
Although occurring primarily in the form of a dyadic relationship, paranoid
delusions have been reported to occasionally occur in larger sect-like groups
whose members become infused with the paranoid ideation of a dominant member.
Norman Cameron termed this a “paranoid pseudocommunity.”97–99 This term is used
to denote an imagined persecutorial conspiracy directed at the group member.
Once separated from the group’s social fabric, many members have been observed
to regain the ability to view others without undue levels of suspicion.100 This
pattern of group behavior may lie along a continuum with that of the
fight–flight group described by Wilfred Bion. Much more speculatively, each of
our subjects and their followers could have been either an initiator or
recipient in a chain of persons who transmit delusional-like beliefs. Each
recipient would then act as the primary case to another individual. No reported
cases of such a chain of transmission of delusion-like beliefs are known to the
authors, and, therefore, this possibility is highly speculative. Generally
speaking, it is an individual’s insight and amenability to reason that are
important means by which sane and psychotic thought processes are distinguished.
A significant limitation of this analysis is that we cannot now know to what
degree the beliefs of our subjects were fostered and maintained within a
cultural “microbubble,” and to what degree their beliefs were amenable to being
changed through reasonable processes.
Last, in response to the proposal that a non-literal interpretation of religious
writings is most advisable, it is observed that, since the earliest of times,
believers have understood our subjects’ experiences as having occurred literally
as described. As such, a great many of these experiences bear a striking
resemblance to well-characterized psychiatric phenomena. This raises the
prospect of an unusual degree of accuracy in the sources with regard to these
details.
Discussion about a potential role for the supernatural is outside the scope of
our article and is reserved for the communities of faithful, religious scholars,
and theologians, with one exception. It is our opinion that a neuropsychiatric
accounting of behavior need not be viewed as excluding a role for the
supernatural. Herein, neuropsychiatric mechanisms have been proposed through
which behaviors and actions might be understood. For those who believe in
omnipotent and omniscient supernatural forces, this should pose no obstacle, but
might rather serve as a mechanistic explanation of how events may have happened.
No disrespect is intended toward anyone’s beliefs or these venerable figures.
If such is perceived after reading this analysis it might be asked whether there
is a stigma in the reader’s mind about mental illness. Any stigma toward persons
with mental illness is rejected by the authors.
Future Directions
Research into this postulated form of psychiatric presentation might be
facilitated by development of a new DSM diagnostic subcategory of schizophrenia
or psychosis and an improved recognition that a continuum of psychotic
symptomatology likely exists.
This subcategory might be referred to as a grandiose or supraphrenic (supra
[above or beyond] phrenic [mind]) variant. It would encompass those who are
symptomatic for 6 months or more, with an organized and relatively nonbizarre
delusional system, grandiosity, often delusional narcissism, possible
hallucinations, and an extremely intense feeling of being supernaturally
selected for a mission. It would recognize that when this occurs in individuals
with generally average-or-higher intelligence, strong communication skills, a
high degree of magnetic charisma, and the ability to effectively engender
empathy, these individuals may be capable of convincing or psychologically
enthralling groups or populations of individuals to follow their directives for
undefined periods of time. Their goals are partly or wholly based on or inspired
by psychotic thought processes. These thought processes may yield beliefs that
are closely related to other common societal beliefs, but they are not very
amenable to reason. Affected individuals may demonstrate a preserved ability to
maintain a social group, be very persuasive, and become socially elevated in a
group and exercise inordinate influence over others in the group. Their beliefs
may result in the sponsorship of activities that are lethal to self and others
and are outside the norms for their society. Disorganization, negative
psychiatric symptoms, and cognitive dysfunction are not significantly present.
Affective features may be present, but are not usually debilitating.
Hyperreligiosity would be a frequent accompaniment, but is not necessarily
required, since extreme devotion to other socio-political belief systems or
perceived extraterrestrial or supernatural forces might serve as surrogates.
These individuals are capable of having extraordinary influence on individuals
and society.
Conclusion
We suggest that some of civilization’s most significant religious figures may
have had psychotic symptoms that contributed inspiration for their revelations.
It is hoped that this analysis will engender scholarly dialogue about the
rational limits of human experience and serve to educate the general public,
persons living with mental illness, and healthcare providers about the
possibility that persons with primary and mood disorder-associated
psychotic-spectrum disorders have had a monumental influence on civilization.
From the Dept. of Neurology, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont,
MA (EDM, BHP); Dept. of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School,
Belmont, MA (MGC); Dept. of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard
Medical School, Belmont, MA (EDM, BHP).
Send correspondence to Evan D. Murray, M.D., Dept. of Neurology, McLean
Hospital, Belmont, MA; e-mail: emurray@mclean.harvard.edu
Written with financial support from the Sidney R. Baer, Jr. Foundation, St
Louis, MO.
Figure Credits: Figure 1: Wikipedia:
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Abraham.jpg; Figure 2: Wikipedia:
http://yo.wikipedia.org/wiki/F%C3%A1%C3%ACl%C3%AC:Domenico_Fetti_004.jpg; Figure
3: Wikipedia:
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Rembrandt_Christ_in_the_Storm_on_the_Lake_of_Galilee.jpg;
Figure 4: Wikipedia Commons: http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:4paul1.jpg.
We thank Steven Schachter, M.D., Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School;
and Jeffrey Rediger, M.D., M.Div., Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, Harvard
Medical School; and The Center for Brain, Law, and Behavior at the Massachusetts
General Hospital, Boston, MA.
References
1
Hazlett SB, McCarthy ML, Londner MS, et al.: Epidemiology of adult psychiatric
visits to U.S. emergency departments. Acad Emerg Med 2004; 11:193–195Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
2
Baillargeon J, Thomas CR, Williams B, et al.: Medical emergency department
utilization patterns among uninsured patients with psychiatric disorders.
Psychiatr Serv 2008; 59:808–811Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
3
Rudaleviciene P, Stompe T, Narbekovas A, et al.: Are religious delusions related
to religiosity in schizophrenia? Medicina (Kaunas) 2008; 44:529–535Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
4
Baier M: Insight in schizophrenia: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2010;
12:356–361Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
5
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric
Association, 2000, pp 297–343Google Scholar
6
Shannon B: Biblical entheogens: a speculative hypothesis. Time and Mind 2008;
1:51–74Crossref, Google Scholar
7
Elliott B, Joyce E, Shorvon S: Delusions, illusions, and hallucinations in
epilepsy, 2: complex phenomena and psychosis. Epilepsy Res 2009;
85:172–186Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
8
Nadkarni S, Arnedo V, Devinsky O: Psychosis in epilepsy patients. Epilepsia
2007; 48(Suppl 9):17–19Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
9
de Araújo Filho GM, da Silva JM, Mazetto L, et al.: Psychoses of epilepsy: a
study comparing the clinical features of patients with focal versus generalized
epilepsies. Epilepsy Behav 2011; 20:655–658Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
10
Ogata A, Miyakawa T: Religious experiences in epileptic patients with a focus on
ictus-related episodes. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 52:321–325Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
11
Kanemoto K, Kawasaki J, Kawai I: Postictal psychoses: in comparison with acute
interictal psychoses. Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol 1994; 48:209–211Medline, Google
Scholar
12
Brewerton TD: Hyperreligiosity in psychotic disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994;
182:302–304Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
13
Landtblom AM, Lindehammar H, Karlsson H, et al.: Insular cortex activation in a
patient with “sensed presence”/ecstatic seizures. Epilepsy Behav 2011;
20:714–718Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
14
Picard F, Craig AD: Ecstatic epileptic seizures: a potential window on the
neural basis for human self-awareness. Epilepsy Behav 2009; 16:539–546Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
15
Asheim Hansen B, Brodtkorb E: Partial epilepsy with “ecstatic” seizures.
Epilepsy Behav 2003; 4:667–673Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
16
Naito H, Matsui N: Temporal lobe epilepsy with ictal ecstatic state and
interictal behavior of hypergraphia. J Nerv Ment Dis 1988; 176:123–124Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
17
Cirignotta F, Todesco CV, Lugaresi E: Temporal lobe epilepsy with ecstatic
seizures (so-called Dostoevsky epilepsy). Epilepsia 1980; 21:705–710Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
18
Prendergast J, Spira P, Schnieden V: Post-ictal psychosis: an unusual clinical
entity. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1999; 33:433–435Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
19
Dongier S: Statistical study of clinical and electroencephalographic
manifestations of 536 psychotic episodes occurring in 516 epileptics between
clinical seizures. Epilepsia 1959/1960; 1:117–142Crossref, Medline, Google
Scholar
20
Kanemoto K: Postictal psychoses, in Progress in Epileptic Disorders:
Neuropsychiatric Issues in Epilepsy. Montrouge, France. Edited by Inoue
YMatsuura M. John Libbey Eurotext, 2010, pp 27–34Google Scholar
21
Umbricht D, Degreef G, Barr WB, et al.: Postictal and chronic psychoses in
patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:224–231Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
22
Kanner AM, Stagno S, Kotagal P, et al.: Postictal psychiatric events during
prolonged video-electroencephalographic monitoring studies. Arch Neurol 1996;
53:258–263Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
23
Trimble M, Kanner A, Schmitz B: Postictal psychosis. Epilepsy Behav 2010;
19:159–161Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
24
Kanemoto K: Postictal psychoses, revisited, in The Neuropsychiatry of Epilepsy.
Edited by Trimble MRSchmitz B. Cambridge, U.K., New York, Cambridge Univ. Press,
2002, pp 117–131Crossref, Google Scholar
25
Devinsky O: Epilepsy: Patient and Family Guide, 3rd Edition. Demos Health, 2007,
pp 20Google Scholar
26
Holmes GL: Epilepsy.com. Temporal Lobe Epilepsy.
http://www.epilepsy.com/epilepsy/epilepsy_temporallobe; last accessed
05/28/2011)Google Scholar
27
Professional Advisory Board: EpilepsyFoundation.org. Temporal Lobe Epilepsy.
http://www.epilepsyfoundation.org/about/types/syndromes/temporallobe.cfm; last
accessed June 4, 2011)Google Scholar
28
Dolgoff-Kaspar R, Ettinger AB, Golub SA, et al: Numinous-like auras and
spirituality in persons with partial seizures. Epilepsia 2011; 52:640–644
10.1111/j.1528- PubMed 1167.2010.02957.x. Epub 2011 Feb 5.Google Scholar
29
Cavanna AE, Cavanna S, Cavanna A: Epileptic seizures and spirit possession in
Haitian culture: report of four cases and review of the literature. Epilepsy
Behav 2010; 19:89–91Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
30
Bortz JJ: Neuropsychiatric and memory issues in epilepsy. Mayo Clin Proc 2003;
78:781–787Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
31
Patrikelis P, Angelakis E, Gatzonis S: Neurocognitive and behavioral functioning
in frontal lobe epilepsy: a review. Epilepsy Behav 2009; 14:19–26Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
32
Hoffmeier, James K: Israel in Egypt: The Evidence for the Authenticity of the
Exodus Tradition. Oxford University Press, 1999Google Scholar
33
Schultze-Lutter F, Ruhrmann S, Hoyer C, et al.: The initial prodrome of
schizophrenia: different duration, different underlying deficits? Compr
Psychiatry 2007; 48:479–488Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
34
Yung AR, McGorry PD: The prodromal phase of first-episode psychosis: past and
current conceptualizations. Schizophr Bull 1996; 22:353–370Crossref, Medline,
Google Scholar
35
Flaherty AW: Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea-generation and
creative drive. J Comp Neurol 2005; 493:147–153Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
36
Flaherty A: The Midnight Disease: The Drive to Write, Writer's Block, and the
Creative Brain. New York, Mariner Books, 2005, p 41Google Scholar
37
Trimble MR. The Soul in the Brain: The Cerebral Basis of Language, Art, and
Belief. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins, 2007, p 101–114Google Scholar
38
Nestor PG: Mental disorder and violence: personality dimensions and clinical
features. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1973–1978Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
39
Hor K, Taylor M: Suicide and schizophrenia: a systematic review of rates and
risk factors. J Psychopharmacol 2010; 24(Suppl):81–90Crossref, Medline, Google
Scholar
40
Meltzer HY: Suicidality in schizophrenia: a review of the evidence for risk
factors and treatment options. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2002; 4:279–283Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
41
Lord V: Suicide by Cop: Inducing Officers to Shoot. New York, Loose Leaf Law
Publications, 2004Google Scholar
42
Keram E, Farrell B: Suicide by Cop: Issues in Outcome and Analysis. Suicide and
Law Enforcement, 2001, pp 587–599Google Scholar
43
Landsborough D: St Paul and temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1987; 50:659–664Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
44
Rieu CH: Introduction, notes, in The Acts of the Apostles. Translated by Rieu
CH. New York, Penguin Classics, 1957, pp 9–39, 133–136, 170–171Google Scholar
45
Pfleiderer O: Lectures on the influence of the Apostle Paul on the development
of Christianity, delivered in London and Oxford, April and May, 1885. Williams
and Norgate, 1897, p 43Google Scholar
46
The New Oxford Annotated Bible with the Apocrypha: Revised Standard Version.
Edited by May HGMetzger BM. New York, Oxford University Press, 1977, pp
1408Google Scholar
47
The Reformation Study Bible New King James Version Sproul RC. Nashville, TN,
Thomas Nelson Publishers, 1995, p 1408Google Scholar
48
Geschwind N: Behavioural changes in temporal lobe epilepsy. Psychol Med 1979;
9:217–219Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
49
Geschwind N: Interictal behavioral changes in epilepsy. Epilepsia 1983; 24(Suppl
1):S23–S30Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
50
Waxman SG, Geschwind N: The interictal behavior syndrome of temporal lobe
epilepsy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1975; 32:1580–1586Crossref, Medline, Google
Scholar
51
Benson DF: The Geschwind syndrome. Adv Neurol 1991; 55:411–421Medline, Google
Scholar
52
Schachter S: Religion and the Brain: Evidence from Temporal Lobe Epilepsy, in
Where God and Science Meet: How Brain and Evolutionary Studies Alter Our
Understanding of Religion, Vol 2. Edited by McNamara P. Westport, CT, Praeger,
2006, pp 171–188Google Scholar
53
Devinsky O, Najjar S: Evidence against the existence of a temporal lobe epilepsy
personality syndrome. Neurology 1999; 53(Suppl 2):S13–S25Medline, Google Scholar
54
Mungas D: Interictal behavior abnormality in temporal lobe epilepsy: a specific
syndrome or nonspecific psychopathology? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;
39:108–111Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
55
Rodin E, Schmaltz S: The Bear-Fedio personality inventory and temporal lobe
epilepsy. Neurology 1984; 34:591–596Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
56
Stark-Adamec C, Adamec RE, Graham JM, et al.: Complexities in the complex
partial seizures personality controversy. Psychiatr J (Univ Ott) 1985;
10:231–236Medline, Google Scholar
57
Stevens JR: Interictal clinical manifestations of complex partial seizures, in
Advances in Neurology. Edited by Penry JKDaly DD. New York, Raven, 1975, pp
85–107Google Scholar
58
Stern A: [The problem of St. Paul’s epilepsy]. Psychiatr Neurol (Basel) 1957;
133:276–284Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
59
Brorson JR, Brewer K: St Paul and temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1988; 51:886–887Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
60
Shimizu M, Kubota Y, Toichi M, et al.: Folie à deux and shared psychotic
disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2007; 9:200–205Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
61
James W: The Varieties of Religious Experience. New York, Barnes & Noble
Classics, 1902, 2004, pp 416–417Google Scholar
62
Karenberg A: Retrospective diagnosis: use and abuse in medical historiography.
Prague Med Rep 2009; 110:140–145Medline, Google Scholar
63
Price BH, Richardson EP: The neurologic illness of Eugene O’Neill: a
clinicopathological report. N Engl J Med 2000; 342:1126–1133Crossref, Medline,
Google Scholar
64
Kahn CRKing GLMoses AC, (eds): Joslin's Diabetes Mellitus, 14th Ed.
Philadelphia, PA, Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins, 2005, p 2Google Scholar
65
Friedman RE. Who Wrote the Bible? HarperOne, January 1, 1987, pp 15–33Google
Scholar
66
Erhman BD. The New Testament: A Historical Introduction to the Early Christian
Writings, 5th Ed. New York, Oxford Univ. Press, 2011Google Scholar
67
Murray ED, Price BH: The neurological examination, in Comprehensive Clinical
Psychiatry, 1st Ed. Edited by Stern TARosenbaum JFFava Met al.. Philadelphia,
PA, Mosby/Elsevier, 2008Crossref, Google Scholar
68
Ovsiew F, Murray ED, Price BH: Neuropsychiatric approach to the psychiatric
inpatient, in Principles of Inpatient Psychiatry, 1st Edition. Edited by Ovsiew
F, Munich R. Baltimore, MD, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2008Google Scholar
69
Legesse B, Price BH, Murray ED: Brain–behavior relations, in Encyclopedia of
Human Behavior, 2nd Edition. Edited by Ramachandran VS. New York, Academic
Press, 2012Google Scholar
70
Murray ED, Buttner N, Price BH: Depression and psychosis in neurological
practice, in Neurology in Clinical Practice, 6th Ed. Edited by Bradley WG,
Daroff RB, Fenichel GM, et al. London, UK, Butterworth Heinemann, 2012Google
Scholar
71
Zubin J, Spring B: Vulnerability: a new view of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol
1977; 86:103–126Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
72
Russo J, Vitaliano PP, Brewer DD, et al.: Psychiatric disorders in spouse
caregivers of care recipients with Alzheimer’s disease and matched controls: a
diathesis-stress model of psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 1995; 104:197–204Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
73
Portin P, Alanen YO: A critical review of genetic studies of schizophrenia, II:
molecular genetic studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1997; 95:73–80Crossref, Medline,
Google Scholar
74
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS): Mental Health: Culture,
Race, and Ethnicity, A Supplement to Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon
General. Rockville, MD, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public
Health Service, Office of the Surgeon General, 2001, p 26Crossref, Google
Scholar
75
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS): Mental Health: A Report
of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD, U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, Public Health Service, Office of the Surgeon General, 1999, pp 269–284Google
Scholar
76
van Os J, Kapur S: Schizophrenia. Lancet 2009; 374:635–645Crossref, Medline,
Google Scholar
77
Smith MS: The Origins of Biblical Monotheism: Israel’s Polytheistic Background
and the Ugaritic Texts. New York, Oxford Univ. Press, 2001, pp 135–157Crossref,
Google Scholar
78
Rediger J: Bipolar disorder and western anosognosia, in The Psychospiritual
Clinician’s Handbook: Alternative Methods for Understanding and Treating Mental
Disorders. Edited by Mijares SKhalsa G. Binghamton, NY, Haworth Reference Press,
2005, pp 205–231Google Scholar
79
Mohr S, Huguelet P: The relationship between schizophrenia and religion and its
implications for care. Swiss Med Wkly 2004; 134:369–376Medline, Google Scholar
80
Kim K, Hwu H, Zhang LD, et al.: Schizophrenic delusions in Seoul, Shanghai, and
Taipei: a transcultural study. J Korean Med Sci 2001; 16:88–94Crossref, Medline,
Google Scholar
81
Vardy MM, Kaplan BM: Christ/messiah delusions revisited: toward an
anthropological definition of religious delusions. Psychoanal Rev 2008; 95:473–487Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
82
Reavis DJ: The Ashes of Waco: An Investigation. Syracuse Univ Press, 1998Google
Scholar
83
Snow RL: Deadly Cults: The Crimes of True Believers. Westport, CT, Praeger,
2003Google Scholar
84
The MI5 Messiah: Why David Shayler Believes He’s the Son of God; Mail Online.
London, UK, Dailymail.co.uk; 2007Google Scholar
85
The Argus, Visit from the “Siberian Jesus” November 9, 2002Google Scholar
86
Akimoto H: The Aum Cult leader Asahara’s mental deviation and its social
relations. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; 60:3–8Crossref, Medline, Google
Scholar
87
Kalchuri B: Meher Prabhu: Lord Meher, The Biography of the Avatar of the Age,
Meher Baba Manifestation, Inc., 1986Google Scholar
88
Hardy R: Confusion surrounds Najaf battle. BBC News, January 31, 2007Google
Scholar
89
Pechey R, Halligan P: The prevalence of delusion-like beliefs relative to
sociocultural beliefs in the general population. Psychopathology 2011; 44:106–115Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
90
Strauss JS: Hallucinations and delusions as points on continua function: rating
scale evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1969; 21:581–586Crossref, Medline, Google
Scholar
91
Ellett L, Lopes B, Chadwick P: Paranoia in a nonclinical population of college
students. J Nerv Ment Dis 2003; 191:425–430Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
92
Kelleher I, Cannon M: Psychotic-like experiences in the general population:
characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychol Med 2011; 41:1–6Crossref,
Medline, Google Scholar
93
Wilner AR: The Spellbinders: Charismatic Political Leadership. New Haven, CT,
Yale University Press, 1984Crossref, Google Scholar
94
Bion WR: Experiences in Groups and Other Papers. London, UK, Tavistock,
1961Crossref, Google Scholar
95
Post JM: Leaders and Their Followers in a Dangerous World: The Psychology of
Political Behavior. Ithaca, NY, Cornell University Press, 2004, pp 187–199Google
Scholar
96
Ghaemi N: A First-Rate Madness: Uncovering the Links Between Leadership and
Mental Illness. New York, Penguin, 2011 Google Scholar
97
Cameron N: The paranoid pseudocommunity. Am J Sociol 1943; 49:37–38Crossref,
Google Scholar
98
Cameron N: Paranoid conditions and paranoia: American Handbook of Psychiatry,
Vol I. New York, Basic Books, 1959aGoogle Scholar
99
Cameron N: The paranoid pseudocommunity revisited. Am J Sociol 1959b; 65:57–61Crossref,
Google Scholar
100
Myers PL: Paranoid pseudocommunity beliefs in a sect milieu. Soc Psychiatry
Psychiatr Epidemiol 1988; 23:252–255Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar
We thank Steven Schachter, M.D., Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School;
and Jeffrey Rediger, M.D., M.Div., Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, Harvard
Medical School; and The Center for Brain, Law, and Behavior at the Massachusetts
General Hospital, Boston, MA.